Clinical Psychology and Psychotherapy
Clin. Psychol. Psychother. 16, 510–522 (2009)
Published online 2 September 2009 in Wiley
InterScience (www.interscience.wiley.com). DOI:
10.1002/cpp.648
The Dodo Bird Verdict—Controversial,
Inevitable and Important: A Commentary on 30
Years of Meta-Analyses
Rick Budd1 and Ian Hughes2*
1 Ludlow Street Healthcare, Llanbedr Court, Newport, Wales, UK
2 Psychology Department, Cardiff and Vale NHS Trust, Cardiff, Wales, UK
The Dodo Bird Verdict—Controversial, Inevitable and Important: A Commentary on 30 Years of Meta-Analyses
Rick Budd1 and Ian Hughes2*
1 Ludlow Street Healthcare, Llanbedr Court, Newport, Wales, UK
2 Psychology Department, Cardiff and Vale NHS Trust, Cardiff, Wales, UK
Abstract
In this article, the assertion that different
psychological therapies are of broadly similar efficacy—often called the ‘Dodo
Bird Verdict’—is contrasted with the alternative view that there are
specifi c therapies that are more effective than others for particular
diagnoses. We note that, despite thirty years of meta-analytic reviews
tending to support
the finding of therapy equivalence, this view is
still controversial and has not been accepted by many within the
psychological therapy community; we explore this from a theory of science
perspective. It is further argued that the equivalence of
ostensibly different therapies is an inevitable consequence of the methodology
that has dominated
this field of investigation; namely, randomised
controlled trials
[RCTs]. The implicit assumptions of RCTs are
analysed and it is
argued that what we know about psychological
therapy indicates that it is not appropriate to treat ‘type of
therapy’ and ‘diagnosis’as if they were independent variables in an
experimental design. It is noted that one logical consequence of this is that
we would not expect RCTs to be capable of isolating effects that are
specific to ‘type of therapy’ and ‘diagnosis’. Rather, RCTs would only
be expected to be capable of identifying the non-specific effects of
covariates, such as
those of therapist allegiance. It is further
suggested that those nonspecific effects that have been identified via meta-analysis are not trivial findings, but rather characterise
important features of psychological therapy.
CONCLUSIONS AND IMPLICATIONS
FOR FUTURE RESEARCH
As has been argued above, the failure of RCTs
(and the meta-analyses of RCTs) to partition out
any specifi c effects of psychological therapy to
different
types of interventions (even when diagnosis
has been controlled for) is attributable to the
fact
that it is exceptionally diffi cult to
operationalize
and control this independent variable. That is to
say that despite rigorous attempts at manualization
the evidence suggests that it is not possible to
ensure that different therapies do not share such a
large proportion of active therapeutic ingredients
as to dilute the power of RCTs to isolate
treatmentspecifi
c effects. Moreover, and perhaps even more
signifi cantly, it is impossible to ensure that
different
interventions do not affect the same psychological
mechanisms of change, thus similarly limiting the
power of RCTs to isolate treatment specifi c
effects.
When these limitations are combined with the fact
that diagnoses are overlapping, ‘fuzzy’ concepts,
which do not adequately control for psychological
factors (e.g., sub-clinical personality features,
etc.)
that are likely to interact with the type of intervention
in determining its effi cacy, it is not surprising
that the Dodo Bird Verdict has survived for more
than 30 years. In short, its survival rests on the
fact
that while RCTs (and the meta-analysis of data
obtained from RCTs) are capable of demonstrating
that psychological therapy as an aggregate is effective,
they are not capable of identifying the specific
elements of therapy that are effective for
different
individuals.
From the research perspective the clarion call
is clear. RCTs do not represent an objective ‘gold
standard’ of research, but rather, are simply one
of many research methodologies, which, like all
methodologies, have their strengths and weaknesses.
When used to test causal relationships
between a dependent variable and a set of independent
variables that can be adequately defi ned
and operationalized, there is no alternative to
constructing
an RCT. However, for addressing other
research questions, such as ‘how does psychological
therapy help different people change?’ different
methodologies could be more appropriate (e.g.,
Pachankis & Goldfried, 2007).
Moreover, there is a need to move away from
designing psychological interventions targeted
at an invalid psychiatric nosology, to developing
interventions directed at alleviating distressing
experiences (e.g., low mood, intrusive memories)
or which are directed at changing problematical
behaviours (e.g., avoidance, rumination). These
interventions need to be developed in light of our
understanding of the psychosocial processes that
maintain them. How and when they are used in
therapy needs to be informed by a full
understanding
of the individual therapy process.
To illustrate our favoured approach, let us
consider
‘depression’. Typically, within the present,
diagnostically driven approach, patients diagnosed
as having ‘depression’ are offered a package—
often of around 20 sessions—of CBT treatment,
which comprises a number of almost invariant
elements
(e.g., activity scheduling, thought diaries,
thought challenging). We consider that such an
approach ignores the variability and complexity
of the symptoms of people diagnosed as having
‘depression’, and that it also ignores the
psychosocial
factors maintaining the symptoms (e.g.,
family relationships, employment problems, thebroader
environmental context); and, that it also
ignores the therapeutic relationship. Low mood,
for instance, is a very common feature of ‘depression’.
In order to ameliorate it, we would advocate
a detailed analysis of its maintaining factors on
an individual basis, and then the application of
an appropriate technique (e.g., increasing activity
levels or resolving
relationship confl icts or
exploring
the validity of negative automatic thoughts).
But, importantly, this needs to be done within
the context of the therapeutic process, that is to
say, at an appropriate time, and in a way that is
most likely to promote change within the client.
(A similar general approach is much more fully
described in the important chapter by Norcross
and Beutler [2008]).
Most importantly we need to reject the medical
model, cease to view therapy as being like a drug
that is given to clients and,
instead, view it for what
it is, a social infl uence process that occurs
(most
typically)
between two people.
No comments:
Post a Comment
PLEASE ADD COMMENTS SO I CAN IMPROVE THE INFORMATION I AM SHARING ON THIS VERY IMPORTANT TOPIC.