Popular Posts

Total Downloads Worldwide

Sunday, 30 June 2013

THE PSYCHOLOGIST - JULY 2013 ISSUE - LETTERS PAGE POWERFUL DEBATE :"Stepping from the shadow of biological reductionism into the light of scientific humanism." - RESPONSES TO THE DIVISION of CLINICAL PYSCHOLOGY (BPS) POSITION STATEMENT ON CLASSIFICATION OF MENTAL HEALTH - JUNE 2013




Let's together remove the life-long burden on young people of the attributional stigmatisation of mental health diagnosis.


We are writing to commend the immediate and comprehensive use of the DCP statement on formulation to all of our colleagues. It has appeared at the very time when the shortcomings of psychiatric diagnosis have been exposed

in the critical international response to the publication of DSM-5 by the American Psychiatric Association. Our current

context then provides the profession of clinical psychology with a unique historical opportunity to adopt a clear position of scientific humanism.


DSM and other forms of psychiatric nosology are incompatible with a psychological approach to helping people with their problems, which should be both humane and scientific. Our approach to helping others should be based on identifying

specific problems (defined by clients themselves but, for obvious

practical purposes reflecting a common lexicon) and working with them to develop individual and context-bound

formulations. These would include the unique events in a person’s life past and present, the meanings they invest in, or

attribute to, those events and strengths to build upon that he or she has exhibited to date in coping with challenges in their

life.


David Pilgrim

Professor of Health and Social Policy

Peter Kinderman

Professor of Clinical Psychology

Richard Bentall

Professor of Clinical Psychology

University of Liverpool


As 210 clinical psychologists and mental health professionals, we support the DCP’s call for a paradigm shift in how

we think about mental distress and the need to move away from

psychiatric diagnosis. We are pleased by the media coverage and the debate this has stimulated in the wider public.


It is essential that diverse voices are heard and that rather than

considering individuals as receptacles of disorders, deficits and distortions, we make sense of distress in more helpful and

evidence-based ways. We need to focus far more on

people’s lives, experiences and social contexts and to consider how people embody and are shaped by the world around them.

We note that organisations such as the Hearing Voices Network

and Mental Health Europe, which represent the service-user perspective, have recently challenged the diagnostic and

professional expert-driven status quo, and we believe

that clinical psychology needs to support and work with these groups. We need to step out from the shadow of biological

reductionism and consider the multifaceted nature of what it is to be human and to be part of the world around us. We wish to

support the DCP and the growing number of service users, carers, professionals and organisations who are questioning the dominant paradigm.


This is a very important step for the profession and one that

is long overdue.


Dr Mel Wiseman

Wellingborough

and 209 other signatories

(For the full list see the html version in Lettersat  www.thepsychologist.org.uk)


Following the publication of the DCP’sPosition Statement on Diagnosis, which we have already endorsed, the Psychosis

and Complex Mental Health Faculty is keen to work on bringing about thechanges in practice and conceptualisation

that it envisages in our services. Most of us in the Faculty work closely with colleagues in other professions in a team

context, so that collaboration, both with them and with service users and carers, is a priority. We are seeking to further this

agenda at the Faculty conference and AGM to be held on 20 November at the BPS London office under the heading

‘Developing the narrative – Creating a shared paradigm shift towards a holistic view of mental health’. This should further the process of moving beyond restrictive, illness-based

conceptualisations in partnership with the other groups and professions concerned with complex mental health, in line with

the second recommendation at the end of the position statement.


Isabel Clarke

Chair of the PCMH Faculty of the DCP on

behalf of the Faculty Committee



We welcome contributions to the debate that has been raised by the DCP Position Statement on Classification, which can be

read in full at http://dcp.bps.org.uk/dcp/ the_dcp/news/dcp-position-statement-onclassification.cfm


The statement is not about DSM specifically but about conceptual systems ‘based on a “disease” model’. This would

include ICD. Nor do we see it as an ‘attack’ but as a thoughtful critique based on a two-year process of reviewing the

evidence and consulting within all the DCP Faculties, where it has widespread support.


It is difficult to convey a complex argument through the media. We regret that some of the reporting has badged this as a psychiatry versus psychology battle.


We have been working hard to counter this. The statement itself makes it absolutely clear that, to quote: ‘This position should not be read as a denial of the role of biology in mediating and

enabling all forms of human experience, behaviour and distress… It recognises the complexity of the relationship between social, psychological and biological factors’ (p.2).

We are unhappy with responses that, quite wrongly, represent us as presenting ‘a false dichotomy between genes and

environment’ and hope that all members of the Society will take the opportunity to correct such misinterpretations.


Nevertheless, our position, supported by a great deal of evidence, is that it is neither accurate nor helpful to conceptualise the experiences that may lead to a functional

psychiatric diagnosis within a ‘disease’ model, in which biological causal factors such as genes or biochemistry are

hypothesised to be the primary causal ones. We do not do this for other responses to life events – for example, bereavement – and by analogy, our argument is that the increasing amount

of evidence for the causal role of all kinds of traumas and life circumstances in psychiatric breakdown makes it implausible to do so in many of these cases as well. The DCP is, in conjunction

with the BPS Media Centre, monitoring the media coverage closely and working to correct any distortions of our message.


The point is well made that we need to explore the possibility of alternative clustering systems, and internationally there are a number of groups engaged in this task. The DCP has funded its own project to outline the principles of an approach that identifies common patterns of responses, both psychological and

biological, to life events and social circumstances, and that might supplement and support the use of individual formulation.


Clearly, the existing classificationsystem will be with us for some time. However, the DCP believes that it is

vitally important to ‘achieve greater openness and transparency about the uses and limitations of the current system’ and ‘to open up dialogue with partner

organisations, service users and carers, voluntary agencies and other professional bodies in order to find agreed ways

forward’ (p.4).


Richard Pemberton

Chair, BPS Division of Clinical Psychology


 Professors Viding and Frith(Letters, June 2013) excoriate
critics of DSM-5 who, they say, ‘are in danger of muddying
mental health issues by ignoring… biology’. They write approvingly of Simon Wessely’s Observer article (12 May 2013) and endorse his claim that ‘a classification system is like a map. And just as any map is provisional, ready to be changed as the
landscape changes, so is classification.’
 


Let me outline how history illustrates the value of this metaphor of mapping the mind.
 

In 1952, when the first Definitely Scientific Map (let’s call it ‘DSM-1’) was published, only 106 cartographic entities
were in the atlas. A good index of the success of the cartographers of the mind is the phenomenal productivity
of their subsequent explorations – new islands, continents, rivers, mountain ranges, swamps, and so on, were added, and although
many old ones were thrown out they achieved an average rate of increase over the next 42 years of close to one every eight weeks: DSMap-4 (1994) showed 365 entities.
 

How had they achieved this fecundity? Untiring effort was the answer. Year in, year out their exploration vessels sailed the seven (7.34 ± 1.56) seas, proudly flying the Cartographers’ flag, a banner inscribed ‘BP’. (Crew members gave different answers when asked what the letters stood for: ‘Big Pharma’, said some,
‘Big-time Psychiatry,’ said others, but the groups worked
together as one big happy family regardless). It was not always smooth sailing. For example, when explorers sent descriptions
of the landscape to the head office of the Cartographers of
the Mind Association (CoMA) for official rulings, one might be told that a tenant was on Mount Skitzos while a second tenant in the same building was on the Isle of Catatonia, a third on Lake Normalia.


But none of this impeded the enlightened help BP could provide to the inhabitants – the holds of the vessels of exploration were brimming with curative chemicals that were equally effective
everywhere on the Map.
 

There were of course sceptics, people who thought, for example, that it might be more effective to build warm buildings in (bi?) polar regions instead of filling inhabitants with chemicals
that made them complain less about the cold. As a gesture of
goodwill (and to try to rid themselves of the distractions
of repetitive complaints), exploration vessels started to carry small amounts of building material, pumps to drain swamps, and so on. CoMA itself even professed to subscribe to a Bio-Psycho-
Social model of disorder: their banners accordingly now read
‘BPS’ not ‘BP’. (This new image has perhaps brought to light
a little-researched version of the Stockholm syndrome, with scientists who are not members of the BP team taking up cudgels on the team’s behalf). Sceptics even argued that the metaphor itself is flawed. Geographic cartographers, they say, draw representations of things we are reasonably sure exist independently of the maps drawn of them: mountains, islands, and so on, they disingenuously claim, are real. DS Maps of mental disorders, they say, are different. They are not
representations of realities: the entities they purport to describe are constructions of the minds of Cartographers of
the Mind. They are Maps of Wonderland.


Professor Justin Joffe
London N2


The American critic H.L. Mencken once remarked, ‘For
every subtle and complicated question, there is a perfectly
simple and straightforward answer, which is wrong’. The
question of how to respond to psychological distress is subtle
and complicated. The answer that has dominated recent
Western thinking, namely that there exist mental illnesses and that these are illnesses like any other, has the virtue of being simple and straightforward. But it may also be wrong – or at least,
partial, misleading and, in some cases, actively unhelpful.
The DCP’s recent statement is to be commended for not trying to replace one ‘simple, straightforward and wrong’ answer with another. The document acknowledges the subtle and complicated
nature of the issues. Contrary to what some commentators
have suggested, it does not pit clinical psychology against
psychiatry or deny the role of biology – indeed, it states
explicitly that what is required is ‘multi-factorial and contextual approach, which incorporates social, psychological and biological factors’. It highlights problems with the current system of classification, but does not object to classification per se.
It offers no off-the-shelf alternative, calling instead for
wide-ranging dialogue to develop new approaches. Even
the document’s most striking letters 468 vol 26 no 7 july 2013
letters suggestion, a move away from the system of diagnosis
described by the DSM-5, is hardly radical. Similar arguments have recently been made by influential and mainstream groups such as Mental Health Europe (see tinyurl.com/bqdgos9) and the
US National Institute for Mental Health (see tinyurl.com/cl5ekbc).
 

That such a measured and non-polemical statement should provoke howls of outrage perhaps tells us something about the tenuous foundations of the medical model. If proponents had the
confidence of their convictions they would have nothing to
fear from – indeed would welcome – critical interrogation. By contrast, those who raise problems with diagnosis, or with the concept of mental illness as such, are accused of ‘anti psychiatry
prejudice’ and of having no interest in relieving suffering
(see tinyurl.com/no88tpb).
 

Most baffling of all is the response that criticising diagnosis is somehow antiscientific – particularly absurd when, as the DCP statement makes clear, many of the difficulties of the DSM arise
from a failure to follow the scientific method.
Although the DCP statement makes no new arguments, it performs a valuable service by bringing vital critiques of the medical model of mental illness to wider public attention.
Personally, I am proud to see the BPS finding its voice and
raising subtle and complex questions.
 

Dr Sam Thompson
Institute for Psychology, Health
and Society
University of Liverpool 



              
Given the importance they attach toevidence, it is surprising that Essi Vidingand Uta Frith (Letters, June) should usethe words ‘attacking the DSM-5’, implying both aggression and lack of justification,for what is in fact a thoughtful, evidence based
statement by the Division of Clinical Psychology (DCP) on psychiatric classification.
In criticising the statement and subsequent ‘pronouncements in the
media’, Viding and Frith present the current situation in mental health as one led by evidence, in which researchersand clinicians take due account of both environmental and biological factors in
understanding ‘the symptoms that mark mental illness’. This is far from the case.
For the last 30 or 40 years, research and practice in this area has been dominated by approaches which privilege genes and
problems as akin to physical illnesses,and systematically de-emphasise the potential causal role of people’s social
and personal contexts. This is in spite of a poor evidence base for all three of these
stances. This situation itself has a social
context, which may help explain both its
persistence and the strength of feeling
often evoked by attempts at change
(Boyle, 2011; Cromby et al., 2013;
Pilgrim, 2007).
And contrary to Viding and Frith’s
claims, the DCP argument is not based
on a ‘false dichotomy between genes and
environment’. It does position itself for a
reconceptualisation of the role of biology
and against a model that sees mental,
emotional and behavioural difficulties as
symptomatic of biologically based illness.
It also argues for due acknowledgement
of the vast amount of evidence that many
of these difficulties are meaningful
responses to often
extremely
challenging life
circumstances.
Finally, Viding
and Frith imply
that those who
claim a causal
link between
child abuse and
‘schizophrenia’ are ‘not slowed down by
a need for an evidence base, but instead
irresponsibly make unsubstantiated and
alarmist pronouncements’. This is
completely unjustified. There is good
evidence, some of it cited in the DCP
statement, that the links between child
abuse and psychosis are likely to be
causal, and such claims are not made
lightly. This evidence may be difficult for
many to hear and unfortunately, Viding
and Frith’s ad hominem response, rather
than one engaging with the evidence, is
not untypical.

I hope the DCP statement will encourage truly informed debate on these issues to the benefit of researchers, clinicians and, above all, service users themselves.

Professor Mary Boyle
University of East London

References
Boyle, M. (2011). Making the world go away, and how
psychology and psychiatry benefit. In Rapley, M.,
Moncrieff, J. & Dillon, J. (Eds.) De-medicalising
misery. London: Palgrave Macmillan.
Cromby, J., Harper, D. & Reavey, P. (2013). Psychology,
mental health and distress. London: Palgrave
Macmillan.
Pilgrim, D. (2007). The survival of psychiatric diagnosis.
 

Saturday, 29 June 2013

TES ARTICLE COINCIDES WITH BPS CONFERENCE WHICH STIMULATED HEALTHY DEBATE ON DSM-5's DANGERS - Boom in ADHD drugs for children is a bitter pill - By Kerra Maddern - COURTESY OF THE TES ONLINE WEBSITE





CLICK ON LINK FOR FULL ARTICLE:

Boom in ADHD drugs for children is a bitter pill

 Published in TES magazine on 28 June, 2013 | By: Kerra Maddern

Experts launch inquiry as funding cuts lead to overprescription

Pressure on local authority and medical budgets is leading to the overprescription of drugs to treat children who have conditions such as attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD), experts have warned.

Psychologists want to see tougher regulations and a significant reduction in the prescription of psychotropic drugs, including Ritalin, after a boom in their use over the past decade. There is evidence that the drugs are being given to children as young as 3, in contravention of health guidelines.

The British Psychological Society (BPS) has launched a major inquiry in the UK, amid fears that cuts in funding for recommended treatments for children with ADHD, including counselling, are leading to the increased prescription of drugs. Meanwhile, in the US, Professor Allen Frances, an expert in mental health diagnosis, warned that excessive use of ADHD drugs had been exacerbated by “aggressive and misleading marketing by drug companies”.

National guidelines in England and Wales say that children with ADHD should receive “comprehensive” treatment, including psychological, behavioural and educational help. But Vivian Hill, chair of the BPS’s medicalisation of childhood working group, told TES that this did not always happen.

“We are responding to a high level of concern with regard to children’s mental health and the prescription of psychotropic drugs,” she said. “Budgets have been cut and psychiatrists feel they can’t follow the official guidelines, which recommend therapy before drugs are prescribed. In the UK, often the first response now is to issue drugs, not offer therapeutic help.”

The group will speak with teachers, psychologists, psychiatrists, parents and young people about the support available for children with mental health needs, including ADHD. The inquiry will also aim to discover why children from poorer backgrounds seem to be more likely to be prescribed drugs.

Official advice says that the drugs are unsuitable for those under the age of 6, but Ms Hill estimates that “hundreds” of younger children are being given medication for ADHD.

Ms Hill, who is director of professional educational psychology training at the University of London’s Institute of Education, said that children as young as 3 were being given the drugs. Without therapy or other support, which addresses the cause of problems, drug treatment could have little impact, she added.

Figures released last year showed that prescriptions of Ritalin in the UK had quadrupled in little over a decade, from 158,000 in 1999 to more than 661,000 in 2010.

In the US, Professor Frances is a former chair of the fourth edition of the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, an influential guide to diagnosing mental health conditions including ADHD (see panel, page 8). He told TES that the use of psychotropic drugs to treat children had got “out of hand”.

“Some of the medication use is necessary but much of it is stimulated by aggressive and misleading marketing by drug companies,” said Professor Frances, emeritus professor at Duke University in North Carolina. “Except in the clearest or most urgent situations, the best policy with kids is watchful waiting to see if things get better on their own. Excessive medication for kids constitutes a public health experiment without any data on long-term outcome and without informed consent.”

Professor Tim Kendall, director of the UK’s National Collaborating Centre for Mental Health, which produces guidance on the treatment of mental health conditions, said parent training programmes, which help to manage ADHD, were being cut.

“There was an increase in access to parent training programmes, but with the squeeze on local council budgets and social care, and the pressure on (National Health Service) budgets, I have become aware that they are now more difficult to access,” he said. “There has been a rise in the use of methylphenidate (Ritalin) on the basis that there hasn’t been much else available.”

Professor Kendall said that the medical profession did not have evidence of the long-term effects on young children who take these drugs, but he thought there was a “significant chance it would cause them damage”.

A spokesperson for the National Institute for Health and Care Excellence, which advises the National Health Service on treatment, said: “Drugs have a role to play in the treatment of ADHD, but psychotherapy is central in its management. These drugs are not recommended as first-line treatments for young people with mild or moderate ADHD. They are recommended as first-line therapy for school-age children and young people with severe ADHD.”

Medical guidance

The Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM) is the standard classification of mental disorders used by mental health professionals in the US, listing every psychiatric disorder recognised by the country’s healthcare system. A fifth edition was released in May.

The National Collaborating Centre for Mental Health in the UK has advised the medical profession not to use DSM-5. Some critics say that it medicalises normal behaviour.

Guidance in the UK says that children diagnosed with mild ADHD do not need drug treatment, but that medics should be prepared to offer parent training or psychological help if needed or if the disorder worsens.