Should
lack of lust be a psychiatric condition?
What happens when we view mental disorders as
exclusively biological?
Published on May 26, 2013 by Christopher Lane, Ph.D. in
Side Effects
In
“Mind Field,” her sharp review of four new books on American psychiatry,
Talitha Stevenson pauses to note:
Only
3 per cent of DSM disorders have any known biological causes. The causes
of the remaining 97 per cent—and this includes depression, anxiety, schizophrenia,
attention
deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD), bipolar
and all personality disorders—are not known. The theory that chemical
imbalances cause mental illness—that a serotonin deficiency causes depression,
for example—is unproven. Billions of research dollars have been spent on trying
to establish a link between neurotransmitters and mental disorder, and the
attempts have failed. For all the scientific terminology, psychiatric diagnoses
are based on subjective judgments.
This
aside in Stevenson’s review article
in the Financial Times appears just after she quotes DSM-III architect
Robert Spitzer telling author Gary Greenberg, with startling honesty, the DSM
“looks very scientific ... it looks like they must know something.”
Stevenson’s
appraisal of The Book of Woe, Greenberg’s compelling account of the
making of DSM-5, includes cogent reviews as well of Saving Normal
by Allen Frances; Cracked: Why Psychiatry Is Doing More Harm Than Good
by James Davies; and Our Necessary Shadow: The Nature and Meaning of Psychiatry by Tom
Burns—books that take as their central focus the reliability and unreliability
of the DSM as a scientific manual.
“Last
month,” she notes, “Thomas Insel, the director of the American National
Institute of Mental Health (NIMH), observed that the DSM was ‘at best, a
dictionary’ and that ‘symptoms alone rarely indicate the best choice of
treatment.’ A later statement, issued jointly with the NIMH’s president-elect,
stressed that the manual remained ‘the key resource’ but still insisted on the
need for a diagnostic system ‘that more directly reflects modern brain
science.’ ”
Appearing
as they did just days before DSM-5 was published, Dr. Insel’s efforts at
playing down the effect of his earlier remarks doubtless gave the APA limited
cover, even if the NIMH’s support for the manual remains tepid at best.
Remember: Insel had described the DSM as suffering from a “lack of
validity.” But fundamental questions remain, including about the reliability of
biomarkers the NIMH hopes to assign the remaining 97 percent of mental
disorders, whose precise biological causes are not known and may never be.
Recall Stevenson, cited above: “Billions of research dollars have been spent on
trying to establish a link between neurotransmitters and mental disorder, and
the attempts have failed.”
If you switch to the New York Times magazine
this weekend, however, you’ll find an article on sex,
the DSM, and pharmacology entitled “Unexcited? There May Be a Pill for
That.” Author Daniel Bergner picks up the story of Hypoactive
Sexual Desire Disorder (HSDD), a carry-over from DSM-IV,
which the updated manual continues to define as "persistently or
recurrently deficient (or absent) sexual/erotic thoughts or fantasies
and desire for sexual activity for a minimum duration of approximately 6 months."
Entirely relatedly, he also tells of the well-funded drive to form and get
approved a new drug called “Lybrido,” which, he says, has been “created to
stoke sexual desire in women.” Lybrido, libido: the marketing
may make them sound interchangeable, but the neurobiology of desire is
far from clear and likely to remain so.
“‘Female
Viagra,’” notes Bergner, “is the way drugs
like Lybrido and Lybridos tend to be discussed. But this is a misconception.
Viagra meddles with the arteries; it causes physical shifts that allow the
penis to rise. A female-desire drug would be something else. It would adjust
the primal and executive regions of the brain. It would reach into the psyche.”
“The
neural networks of eros,” he adds of the current state of the research, “are
only vaguely known. Tiny subregions and crucial pathways in the brain have been
identified — blurrily, speculatively. Some progress has been made by looking at
what sites in the brain light up when people watch slide shows of pornography
while lying
in magnetic-resonance-imaging cylinders. But the images just aren’t exact
enough. The brain’s interwoven networks are too intricate for the technology to
properly view them.”
In
short, and however unhappily for many, “desire resists comprehension.” “Whether
it is mainly a raw drive or a complex emotion is a question that has bedeviled
psychiatry for decades," he notes. "And the fading of desire can seem
impossibly intricate. Is it a result of a lack of intimacy or its cause?”
Bergner’s
article barely touches on the controversy surrounding the approval and settings
of HSDD, whose principal criterion he sees as sufficiently
open-ended to include “lack of lust, when it creates emotional distress."
But he usefully cautions of both the blindspots in biological psychiatry and
the ongoing bid to medicate female desire: “Millions of American women are on
SSRI” antidepressants, one of whose known side effects is, quite unambiguously,
the “chemical dulling” of their libido.
Such
is our cultural fixation on medication that the subject—and science—of natural aphrodisiacs
isn't even mentioned in the article. Nor are alternative ways of increasing
libido, including by physical exercise.
Bergner is more astute in predicting the likely psychological consequences on
couples if the new drug is approved:
Chemically
enhancing a woman's desire might play out in all kinds of ways within a
relationship. Some couples might feel closer, others might feel desolate
because, despite more sex, their bond isn’t stronger. Wives might yearn for the
old seductive efforts of their husbands, even if those gestures stopped working
long ago. Women might feel yet more pressure to perform: Why not get that
prescription? their partners might ask; why not take that pill? And men, if
they are willing to confront the truth, might not be so happy about the
reminder, as their partners reach for the pill bottle, that their women need
chemical assistance to want them.
Bergner
closes by focusing on one of the participants in the trials, a “44-year-old
part-time elementary-school teacher," who “seemed untroubled by … the
paradox that one of the problems this medication might be addressing is the
desire-killing side effect of yet another type of psychotropic chemical, the
S.S.R.I.’s.”
“She
said that if this drug didn’t work, she would sign up for the next experimental
medication that came along. … For her, the existence of the antidepressants
that so many others take was proof that her problem would be solved.”
Christopher
Lane, Ph.D., teaches literature and intellectual history at
Northwestern University and is the author of Shyness: How Normal Behavior
Became a Sickness. more...
Subscribe to Side Effects
No comments:
Post a Comment
PLEASE ADD COMMENTS SO I CAN IMPROVE THE INFORMATION I AM SHARING ON THIS VERY IMPORTANT TOPIC.